当前位置:首页 » 考试培训 » 雅思专区 » 阅读
2012年9月15日雅思阅读考试机经回忆(二)
来源:毕达教育 发布时间:2012-09-28 17:28:24
下面是2012年9月15日雅思阅读考试机经回忆的内容,大家可以参考一下2012年9月15日雅思阅读考试的内容,更好的备考下一次的雅思阅读考试。
Reading Passage 2
Title:The Playful Nature of Scientists
Question types:
信息段落配对 Which paragraph contains the following information?
多项选择(选两个)
填空
文章内容回顾:关于科学家的幽默的行为。讲述玩乐和科学之间的相同性,探索科学家的玩乐精神。本文难度系数大。
英文原文阅读
I've been working lately on a ludic theory of human nature. In case you haven't studied Latin in a while (perhaps not since several lifetimes ago), I hereby inform you that ludic means playful. I'm calling my theory a ludic theory because if I called it a playful theory you wouldn't take it seriously. (I'm trying hard to ignore the fact that the only common English derivative of ludic is ludicrous.)
Heaven take pity on those few of us who try to take play seriously. It's hard to do. Play, by definition, is something that is not serious. I'm sure that's part of the reason why most serious scholars stay far away from the topic.
The great classic scholarly book on human play is entitled Homo Ludens, which means literally Man the Player. It was written by Johan Huizinga, a Dutch historian, in 1938. It's a wonderful book and has inspired me greatly. But my own theory is quite different from Huizinga's.
Huizinga stated clearly that his is a cultural theory of play, not a biological theory. My theory, in contrast, is fundamentally biological, though it is also cultural, because, in matters of human behavior, biology and culture are inextricably entwined. Another big difference is that Huizinga tended to equate play with contest and to focus on agonistic, or competitive aspects of play, while I hold that play is fundamentally noncompetitive. I can understand how someone such as Huizinga, steeped in Western cultural history, might view play primarily as contest. In my theory, contest is a morphing of play with something that is close to the opposite of play--a drive to beat and dominate others. When we combine these two opposites, play becomes more serious (and thereby more acceptable to contemporary adults) and domination becomes more playful--not entirely a bad thing, but not the same as pure play.
In the remaining paragraphs here, I present a sketch of the ludic theory. In subsequent weekly posts I shall elaborate on specific aspects of the theory, presenting evidence along the way. [Some of what I shall present overlaps with ideas I published in a recent article-- Play as a Foundation for Hunter-Gatherer Social Existence, in The American Journal of Play, 1 (#4), 2009, pp 476-522.]
In most non-human mammals, play occurs almost entirely among the young of the species and seems clearly to serve the function of skill learning and practice. As I have noted in previous posts, young mammals, in play, practice the very skills that they must develop in order to make it into adulthood and to thrive and reproduce. Predators practice predation, as when tiger clubs stalk and pounce on bugs, wind-blown leaves, and each other. Prey animals practice getting away from predators, as when zebra colts dodge and dart in their playful frolicking and endless games of tag. Young males of many species practice fighting, taking turns pinning one another in their species-specific ways and getting out of pinned positions. Young females of at least some species practice nurturance, in playful care of young.
We humans have inherited the basic youthful play characteristics of our animal ancestors, but in the course of our biological and cultural evolution we have elaborated upon them and created new functions. Playfulness in humans does not end when adulthood begins and it serves many functions beyond the learning of species-specific skills.
Social play in all animals requires that all tendencies toward aggression and dominance be suppressed. This is especially true in playful fighting, which is one of the most common forms of animal play. The fundamental difference between a play fight and a real fight is that the former involves no intention to hurt, drive away, or dominate the other animal. A play fight between two young animals can only occur if both are willing partners. Anything that smacks of true aggression or tendency to dominate would cause the threatened animal to run away, and the play, with all its fun and opportunity for learning, would end. And so, in the course of natural selection, animals developed signals to let each other know that their playful attacks are not real attacks, and they developed, for purposes of play, self-restraints and means of self-handicapping to operate against any tendencies to dominate or hurt one another in play.
We inherited these play-enabling signals and restraints from our primate ancestors, and then--through both culture and biological evolution--we built upon them. We brought playfulness and signals associated with it (such as laughter) into adulthood, and we used them to promote ways of cooperating and sharing with one another that surpass those of other mammals.
I am going to argue, in my next post, that when we bring playfulness to bear in our social interactions we create a spirit of equality and personal freedom that allows us to overcome our equally human drive to dominate one another. Hunter-gatherer societies were especially successful in cultivating playfulness as a means of defeating aggression and dominance. Their way of life required close cooperation and sharing, of the sort that could easily be defeated by aggression and dominance. Their playful approach to social life apparently enabled them to survive, relatively peacefully, for hundreds of thousands of years prior to the invention of agriculture. In our culture today, play and humor are still forces for defeating aggression, dominance, and hierarchy, though we don't use them as effectively as hunter-gatherers did.
Play, in any species, is done primarily for the fun of it, not to fill some felt survival need. A young animal or child playing may be learning, but it is not consciously learning; it is just having fun. I don't know if other animals have a perceptual sense of beauty, but it is easy to imagine how doing something just for the fun of it could, in humans, become doing something just for the beauty of it.
Play is also, by definition, creative. It is not an automatic response to demands from outside, but is creative behavior deriving from within. Moreover, play is representative. A play fight is not a fight, but it represents a fight. Playful predation is not a hunt, but it represents a hunt. In humans, the representative power of play grew immensely. Human children--and adults, too--can represent not just fights and hunts, but truly anything in play. Play thereby provides a foundation for all of imagination.
Fun, beauty, creativity, representation, imagination--these are the essences of art, music, literature, theoretical science, and (I will argue two weeks from now) religion. These activities, which characterize our species everywhere, make us human. They all originated biologically in play. Play is the biological germ, which we inherited from our animal ancestors, which grew in us to make us human.
In animals, play is quite separate from productive behaviors. Playful predation and real predation are two different things. But in humans playfulness can blend with productivity. When productive work is suffused with the qualities of play--that is, with freedom, creativity, and imagination--we experience that work as play. Hunter-gatherers had a genius for keeping their productive work within the realm of play. In our culture today, those people who have the most freedom of choice and opportunity for creativity within their work are most likely to say they enjoy their work and regard it as play.
This final point, drawn out, provides the most direct and clear functional line between animal and human play. But education in humans is far more than learning in other species. We are the cultural being, and education is the passing of culture from generation to generation. In previous posts I have already written about play as a vehicle for children's education, but I will have more to say in a future post about the ways by which animal play was modified, in humans, to become such a powerful force for education.
题型难度分析
这篇文章的难度比第一篇的难度略高,除了细节配对和填空题之外,还有多选题,建议先做填空。
题型技巧分析
段落细节信息配对题
1. 无序
2. 注意有可能出现NB
3. 注意大量题目和原文的近义替换
段落细节配对难度较大,建议考生放在本篇文章所有题型的最后去做。做时注意切不可逐题去原文整篇文章搜寻答案,这样会导致文章来来回回看很多遍,耗时太长。
1. 划出所有题目的keywords, 同时考虑到有可能出现近义替换的词,有针对性的去原文寻找答案。比如:看到be conscious of立刻想到雅思高频近义替换是be aware of…, 看到reproduce想到copy。
2. 某些题目可以对题目进行细致的分析。平时通过精读多多熟悉文章结构安排,了解行文模式)
3. 做题时以文章为基准,每看一段,浏览题目中的keywords是否与其相关。
剑桥雅思推荐原文练习剑4 Play is A Serious Business
以上就是关于2012年9月15日雅思阅读考试机经回忆的全部内容。大家可以在备考雅思阅读考试的过程中根据自己的实际情况选择一些话题进行背景知识的准备。预祝大家考试成功。
相关文章
- 揭秘:雅思口语考试5条考生不知道的评分标准...
- 提高雅思口语的实用性技巧
- 雅思口语15个常见媒体话题
- 雅思口语考试中如何巧答考官提问?...
- 针对雅思口语考试进行语音纠错的10个技巧...
- 考生备考雅思听力常见误区总结
- 剖析雅思听力考试的四大特征
- 雅思写作低分原因归总
- 雅思阅读Heading解题技巧讲解
- 雅思阅读Summary题型做题方法详解
- 雅思阅读TFNG题型的做题技巧
- 雅思阅读多项选择题答题技巧解析
- 雅思阅读简答题解题技巧
- 雅思阅读如何审题?
- 针对雅思阅读选择题出题方式进行解析...
- GRE阅读考试的技巧讲解
- 12月18日托福考试机经回忆
- 2012年1月8日首场托福考试机经回忆
- 2012年2月17日托福考试机经回忆
- 雅思听力机经2008Beta1_毕达学府
- 学会使用雅思机经只要四招搞定_毕达学府...
- 2012年4月21日新GRE考试数学机经回忆...
- 2012年4月21日新GRE考试语文机经回忆...
- 2012年5月12日托福考试机经回忆
相关推荐
- 如何提高雅思写作能力?
- 如何提升雅思作文质量?
- 如何写雅思大作文的结尾?
- 如何写雅思大作文的开头?
- 雅思大作文三大评分标准解析
- 雅思大作文开头写作模板句
- 雅思大作文失分的六个原因
- 雅思大作文写作技巧解析
- GRE阅读理解的三个层次
- GRE阅读五个境界全面突破
- GMAT阅读新技巧之“画”出整体框架...
- GAMT阅读应看重什么——熟悉套路VS看懂文章...
- GMAT阅读,“记住”就能拿高分
- GMAT阅读技巧之略读
- GMAT阅读,抓住规律很关键
- GMAT阅读提高速度——取舍是关键
- 2012年6月16日新GRE考试写作机经回忆...
- 2012年6月16日新GRE考试数学机经回忆...
- 2012年6月16日新GRE考试阅读机经回忆...
- 2012年6月30日托福口语考试机经回忆...
- 2012年6月30日托福写作考试机经回忆...
- 2012年6月30日托福听力考试机经回忆...
- 2012年6月16日雅思听力考试机经回忆...
- 2012年6月16日雅思写作考试机经回忆...

![]() |
毕达北京:010-82486303 |
毕达广州:020-87566672 | 毕达上海:021-6390 6767 |

更多资料下载
